Rhythms
Objectives
- Be aware of the behavioural and cellular processes in which rhythms are manifested.
- Understand the basic circuitry that could be involved in learning
- Know the terms used and display methods used in describing rhythms
- Define circadian rhythms and how to determine whether rhythms are circadian
- Neural basis of rhythms
- Peripheral and central rhythms
Topic outline
- Periodicity and rhythms
- Circadian Rhythms
- Gated Rhythms
Activities
- Minilecture: Rhythms
Periodicity
Many activities of insects—such as feeding, locomotion, mating, oviposition, eclosion, etc.—occur at regular intervals.
Periodicity can be tidal, daily, lunar, annual, multi-annual. It is governed by endogenous and exogenous components.
Rhythms
Rhythms are processes that are governed by an innate “biological clock”. They characteristically continue when external conditions are artificially kept constant. Under constant conditions, the endogenous rhythm is “free running” and shows a periodicity close to normal.
Why should an insect develop a way of maintaining an innate rhythm when the external cues are invariant?
Minilecture:RhythmsPresented by D. Merritt
|
Circadian rhythms
Circadian rhythms have a periodicity of approximately 24 hours. In normal situations they are constantly entrained by environmental cues such as day length. In experimental situations of constant light (LL) or constant dark (DD), they show their inherent “free-running” rhythm of approximately 24 hours. It usually varies between individuals from 22-27 hours. Circadian rhythms are temperature-compensated. We don’t know how this is achieved.
Zeitgeber: an external time cue.
Some rhythms require a zeitgeber for a rhythm to be manifested. For example, mosquitoes kept in constant light will oviposit randomly: after a period of darkness they will oviposit rhythmically at 24 hr intervals. There is an innate ability to “count off” an approximation of 24 hours. And oviposition is gated to a certain period in the cycle.
Genetic basis of rhythmic activity
Drosophila mutants were isolated that had altered pupal eclosion rhythms. Hundreds of individual stocks were generated from individuals that had been exposed to mutagen. Progeny were also assessed for locomotory rhythms. Several alleles of a single gene were isolated. The mutation is called period (per).
--per0 |
has no obvious rhythm |
--perl |
has a longer than normal rhythm |
--pers |
has a shorter than normal rhythm. |
Beside the daily rhythm, mating is also affected. Males sing to females by vibrating their wings. The interpulse song interval for:
--wild type males |
50-65 seconds |
--pers |
35-45 sec |
--perl |
75-95 sec |
There are interspecific song differences between D. melanogaster and D. simulans.
The D. simulans per gene was placed in a transposable P element and used to transform a D. melanogaster per0 recipient. Rhythmicity was restored, but the host (D. simulans) rhythmicity was manifested. Only a few amino acid substitutions are responsible.
per protein is expressed in a subset of neurons in the CNS as well as the antennae, photoreceptors, ovaries. In some tissues this can be related to function: daily turnover of photoreceptor membrane, possible turnover of olfactory membrane.
The glow-worm example
Record of the light output of a glow-worm over approximately 30 days in the laboratory under LD conditions.
Glow-worm bioluminescence is nocturnal.
Are they responding to environmental cues or are they responding to an internal clock?
Best way to distinguish between possibilities is to place in constant dark (D:D), or constant light (L:L) and observe behaviour
Glow-worm bioluminescence “drifts” when placed in constant darkness (DD)
Very characteristically not exactly 24 hours, and there is a day-to-day similar trend
The fact that it is not exactly 24 hr cycle indicates it is obeying an internally-generated rhythm
Approximately 24 hours = “circa” about, “dian” a day: circadian
They show true circadian rhythmicity
Free-running period > 24 h
Rainforest Arachnocampa flava tested in lab
1: exposed to DD to free-run
2. Exposed to LD to show L represses biolum
3. On return to DD the light has re-entrained the rhythms to match the dark phase.
All species:
light is a masking agent, i.e. it blocks bioluminescent output
Light is an entraining agent
In caves (A. tasmaniensis) cycle but don’t free-run. Exposure to artificial light masks and re-entrains to the light cycle.
Lab experiments with weak point-sources show same synchronisation but light not strong enough to mask.
Conclusion: Synchronise to each others’ glows
Gated rhythms
A gated rhythm is something that happens only once in individual’s life. For example, eclosion of Drosophila usually occurs around dawn. If an individual misses a “window of opportunity” on a given day, it must wait until the same time on the next day to emerge. Permits synchronicity in the population, utilisation of resources, escape from predators.
Pupal eclosion rhythms are common. In some insects, for example, drosophilids, the pupa remains light-sensitive, i.e. the eclosion rhythm can be entrained by light in DD. Others, e.g. Sarcophaga and Dacus are insensitive as pupae. Entrainment occurs in larvae.
Time of eclosion of 2 spp of silkmoth is gated at different parts of the daily cycle.
- Brainless animals lose the periodicity (arrhythmic).
- Brain reimplant into abdomen restores periodicity.
- Brain transplant switched the eclosion time to that of the brain donor species.
TOPIC REVIEWDo you know…?
|